Rutter M, Pickles A. 1991. Person-environment interactions: Concepts, mechanisms, and implications for data analysis. In: Wachs TD, Plomin R, eds. Conceptualization and Measurement of Organism Environment Interaction. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Pp. 105–141.


Schneider ML, Roughton EC, Koehler AJ, Lubach GR. 1999. Growth and development following prenatal stress exposure in primates: An examination of ontogenetic vulnerability. Child Dev 70:263–274.

Scott KG, Shaw KH, Urbano JC. 1994. Developmental epidemiology. In: Friedman SL, Haywood HC, eds. Developmental Follow-up. Concepts, Domains, and Methods. San Diego: Academic Press. Pp. 351–374.

Scott KG, Avchen RN, Hollomon HA. 1999. Epidemiology of child developmental problems: The extent of the problems of poor development in children from deprived backgrounds. Food Nutr Bull 20:34–44.

Singer LT, Arendt R, Minnes S, Farkas K, Salvator A, Kirchner HL, Kliegman R. 2002. Cognitive and motor outcomes of cocaine-exposed infants. J Am Med Assoc 287:1952–1960.

Sostek AM. 1992. Prematurity as well as intraventricular hemorrhage influence development outcome at 5 years. In: Friedman SL, Sigman MD, Sigel IE, eds. The Psychological Development of Low-Birthweight Children. Annual Advances in Applied Developmental Psychology. Vol 6. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing. Pp. 259–274.

Spear LP, Campbell J, Snyder K, Silveri M, Katovic N. 1998. Animal behavior models. Increased sensitivity to stressors and other environmental experiences after prenatal cocaine exposure. Ann N Y Acad Sci 846:76–88.

Spencer PS. 1990. Chemical time bombs: Environmental causes of neurodegenerative diseases. In: Russell RW, Flattau PE, Pope AM, eds. Behavioral Measures of Neurotoxicity: Report of a Symposium. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Pp. 268–284.

Steering Committee of the Physicians’ Health Study Research Group. 1989. Final report on the aspirin component of the ongoing Physicians’ Health Study. N Engl J Med 321:129–135.

Strom BL, Schinnar R, Ziegler EE, Barnhart KT, Sammel MD, Macones GA, Stallings VA, Drulis JM, Nelson SE, Hanson SA. 2001. Exposure to soy-based formula in infancy and endocrinological and reproductive outcomes in young adulthood. J Am Med Assoc 286:807–814.


Wachs TD. 2000. Necessary but Not Sufficient. The Respective Roles of Single and Multiple Influences on Individual Development. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Wasserman GA, Liu X, Popovac D, Factor-Litvak P, Kline J, Waternaux C, LoIacono N, Graziano JH. 2000. The Yugoslavia Prospective Lead Study: Contributions of prenatal and postnatal lead exposure to early intelligence. Neurotoxicol Teratol 22:811–818.

Waterland RA, Garza C. 1999. Potential mechanisms of metabolic imprinting that lead to chronic disease. Am J Clin Nutr 69:179–197.

Weiss B. 1995. Incipient hazards of cocaine: Lessons from environmental toxicology. In: Lewis M, Bendersky M, eds. Mothers, Babies, and Cocaine: The Role of Toxins in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Pp. 41–55.

Willoughby A, Graubard BI, Hocker A, Storr C, Vietze P, Thackaberry JM, Gerry MA, McCarthy M, Gist NF, Magenheim M, Berendes H, Rhoads GG. 1990. Population-based study of the developmental outcome of children exposed to chloride-deficient infant formula. Pediatrics 85:485–490.

Winneke G. 1990. Neurobehavioral toxicity of selected environmental chemicals: Clinical and subclinical aspects. In: Russell RW, Flattau PE, Pope AM, eds. Behavioral Measures of Neurotoxicity: Report of a Symposium. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Pp. 226–242.



The National Academies | 500 Fifth St. N.W. | Washington, D.C. 20001
Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved.
Terms of Use and Privacy Statement